Skip to main content

Effects of test experience, closed-arm wall color, and illumination level on behavior and plasma corticosterone response in an elevated plus maze in male C57BL/6J mice: a challenge against conventional interpretation of the test

Abstract

The elevated plus maze test is a widely used test for assessing anxiety-like behavior and screening novel therapeutic agents in rodents. Previous studies have shown that a variety of internal factors and procedural variables can influence elevated plus maze behavior. Although some studies have suggested a link between behavior and plasma corticosterone levels, the relationships between them remain unclear. In this study, we investigated the effects of experience with a battery of behavioral tests, the wall color of the closed arms, and illumination level on the behavior and plasma corticosterone responses in the elevated plus maze in male C57BL/6J mice. Mice were either subjected to a series of behavioral tests, including assessments of general health and neurological function, a light/dark transition test, and an open field test, or left undisturbed until the start of the elevated plus maze test. The mice with and without test battery experience were allowed to freely explore the elevated plus maze. The other two independent groups of naïve mice were tested in mazes with closed arms with different wall colors (clear, transparent blue, white, and black) or different illumination levels (5, 100, and 800 lx). Immediately after the test, blood was collected to measure plasma corticosterone concentrations. Mice with test battery experience showed a lower percentage of open arm time and entries and, somewhat paradoxically, had lower plasma corticosterone levels than the mice with no test battery experience. Mice tested in the maze with closed arms with clear walls exhibited higher open arm exploration than mice tested in the maze with closed arms with black walls, while there were no significant differences in plasma corticosterone levels between the different wall color conditions. Illumination levels had no significant effects on any measure. Our results indicate that experience with other behavioral tests and different physical features of the maze affect elevated plus maze behaviors. Increased open arm time and entries are conventionally interpreted as decreased anxiety-like behavior, while other possible interpretations are considered: open arm exploration may reflect heightened anxiety and panic-like reaction to a novel situation under certain conditions. With the possibility of different interpretations, the present findings highlight the need to carefully consider the test conditions in designing experiments and drawing conclusions from the behavioral outcomes in the elevated plus maze test in C57BL/6J mice.

Introduction

The elevated plus maze test has been widely used to measure anxiety-like behavior and evaluate potential therapeutic agents for anxiety-like traits in rats and mice [1,2,3,4,5,6]. This test is based on an approach/avoidance conflict resulting from the natural tendency of rodents to approach and avoid a potentially dangerous situation. Rodents normally avoid open arms in an elevated plus maze that are considered to be dangerous and instead tend to stay in closed arms for a longer time, and increased exploration of open arms is typically interpreted as a low level of anxiety. It is assumed that the behaviors can be induced by novelty, the presence of open spaces, and height in the testing environment [1, 6,7,8].

Elevated plus maze behavior can be influenced by a variety of internal factors and procedural variables, such as strain, sex, age, apparatus construction, and prior experience [8]. Some researchers have used a test/retest protocol with an intertest interval of at least one day and reported that prior experience with an elevated plus maze test reduces the exploration of open arms in the retest session [7, 9,10,11,12,13,14,15], whereas other reports indicate no changes in the exploration of open arms regardless of prior maze experience [1, 2, 16,17,18]. The decreased exploration of open arms during re-exposure to the maze is hypothesized to be involved in learning and memory of spatial information for open arms that are potentially dangerous [12, 13, 15, 19,20,21], locomotor habituation [10], and an altered receptor function [22, 23]. However, the effects of prior maze experience on subsequent behavior were not affected by manipulations of extramaze cues in DBA/2 mice [12], suggesting no importance of acquiring information about the test environment. In addition, previous reports showed that Swiss mice subjected to a holeboard test immediately before being tested in the elevated plus maze exhibited increased exploration of open arms [2], while repeated handling and exposure to an open field induced reduced exploratory behavior in the elevated plus maze in C57BL/6JOlaHs mice [24]. These findings suggest that such behavioral changes in the elevated plus maze may not necessarily require the learning and memory processes for previous experience in the same maze and can be induced by experience with different testing situations.

Wall transparency of closed arms is one of the factors for apparatus construction influencing elevated plus maze behavior. For example, it has been reported that higher open arm time and entries were observed in a maze with closed arms with transparent walls than with opaque walls in rats [25,26,27] and mice [28]. However, the effects of different color features of transparent walls or opaque walls on behavior have not been well studied. Illumination intensity in the testing environment has also been reported to be a significant factor that could affect open arm exploration. Rodents tested under higher illumination conditions showed reduced exploratory behavior in open arms or increased anxiety-like behavior compared with lower illumination conditions [23, 29,30,31,32], whereas some studies have shown that illumination levels did not affect the behavior [33, 34]. Thus, the effects of illumination on behavior remain inconclusive.

Corticosterone release resulting from hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis activation is necessary for homeostatic maintenance of the central and peripheral nervous systems to stressful or challenging situations [35]. Corticosterone levels have been considered to be associated with elevated plus maze behavior [1, 36,37,38]. In fact, the plasma corticosterone stress response in animals placed in the elevated plus maze is higher than that of animals left undisturbed in their home cages [36,37,38], and confinement to the open arms induces a greater increase in plasma corticosterone than confinement to the closed arms in rats [1]. These reports suggest that exposure to aversive stimuli from a novel situation, open space, and/or height causes a higher stress response. However, a previous study reported that plasma corticosterone levels showed no correlations with open arm exploration but significant correlations with risk-assessment behavior in Swiss-Webster mice that were allowed to freely explore an elevated plus maze [39]. Thus, little is still known about the relationships between plasma corticosterone levels and elevated plus maze behavior.

C57BL/6 mice are one of the most commonly used inbred mouse strains in behavioral and neuroscience research. Despite the extensive use of this strain, the influences of internal factors and procedural variables, e.g., experience with other behavioral tests and different physical features of the apparatus, on elevated plus maze behavior and plasma corticosterone response to the elevated plus maze have not been well studied in C57BL/6J mice. It is thus important to understand the influences of various experiences before the elevated plus maze test and apparatus conditions on elevated plus maze behavior and corticosterone response in this strain of mice. Additionally, it is interesting to explore whether corticosterone levels reflect “anxiety” levels that are naturally induced by exposure to potentially aversive situations in the elevated plus maze in C57BL/6J mice. In the present study, to examine the influences of experience with a battery of behavioral tests that did not include the elevated plus maze test on subsequent elevated plus maze behavior and plasma corticosterone responses in male C57BL/6J mice, animals were first subjected to a behavioral test battery, including an assessment of general health and neurological reflexes, a light/dark transition test, and an open field test, which have been extensively used to characterize the behavioral phenotypes of many strains of mutant and inbred mice in our laboratory [e.g., 4043] and in other laboratories [e.g., 4447]. Mice with and without experience with the behavioral test battery were tested in the elevated plus maze, and immediately after exposure to the maze, their blood was collected to measure plasma corticosterone levels. Additionally, to further explore the relationships between elevated plus maze behavior and plasma corticosterone levels after exposure to the elevated plus maze, the present study examined the influence of different test conditions, i.e., closed arm wall color and illumination level [25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34], on behavioral and endocrine responses.

Methods

Animals

Male C57BL/6J mice at 46–54 days old (n = 132 in total, from 4 different cohorts) were transported from Charles River Laboratories Japan, Inc. to our animal facility. The C57BL/6J strain has been maintained under The Jackson Laboratory's patented Genetic Stability Program at Charles River Laboratories Japan, Inc. After arrival at our facility, the mice were housed in groups (four per cage) in clear-transparent individually ventilated disposable cages (Innocage, 22.7 cm × 32.3 cm × 12.7 cm; Innovive, Inc., San Diego, USA) with paper bedding (PaperClean; Japan SLC, Inc., Shizuoka, Japan), which were placed in the middle or lower shelf of the rack in a ventilated rack system (Innorack; Innovive, Inc., San Diego, USA). The room was illuminated with fluorescent ceiling lights with a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 AM). The illumination level in the cage in the middle shelf was approximately 25 lx. The cage position within the rack was kept through this study. The room temperature was maintained at 23 ± 2 °C. The mice were given access to food (CRF‐1; Oriental Yeast Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) and water ad libitum throughout the study. The cages and paper bedding were changed every 10–14 days but not changed before and during the behavioral tests to avoid disturbing the mice. Animal care was carried out by a female laboratory technician. All experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Fujita Health University.

The first cohort of mice (n = 32) was randomly assigned to either a test-battery-experience group or a no-test-experience group (n = 16 per group). Mice in the test-battery-experience group were subjected to a series of behavior tests that were performed in the following order: assessment of general health and neurological screening (2-day procedure; the day of start of the test battery was designated as test day 1; mice were 72–78 days old on test day 1 and 73–79 days old on test day 2), light/dark transition test (75–81 days old on test day 4), and open field test (77–83 days old on test day 6), with 2-day intertest intervals. The tests were performed in the same manner and the same order as previously described [e.g., 4043; for details, see the “Behavioral test” section]. The no-test-experience group was left undisturbed until the start of the elevated plus maze test. Two to 4 days after the open field test (79–87 days old on test days 8–10), the mice of both groups were tested in the elevated plus maze, as described below. On each day of testing, the elevated plus maze test was performed in a manner counterbalanced for the groups to test approximately the same number of mice from each test condition (for details, see Additional File 1: Data file). Immediately after the elevated plus maze test, blood was collected from all the animals (for details, see the “Plasma corticosterone measurement” section).

The second cohort of naïve mice (n = 48) was tested at 73–82 days old in the elevated plus maze with closed arms of different colors (n = 12 per closed-arm color test condition): clear, transparent blue (the same color as in the experiment for the first cohort), white, and black (the first two were transparent and the last two were opaque; see Additional File 2: Fig. 1a and b). The test was conducted in the same manner as the experiment for the first cohort for 4 days. On each day, the test was performed in a manner counterbalanced for the test condition to test the same number of mice from each condition (for details, see Additional File 1: Data file), and then blood was collected immediately after the elevated plus maze test.

The third cohort of naïve mice (n = 36) was subjected to the elevated plus maze test under three different illumination levels: 5, 100, and 800 lx on the central platform in the maze (n = 12 per test condition, 72–80 days old). In this test, the same transparent blue closed arms as those in the experiment with the first cohort were used. For 3 days, the test was performed in a manner counterbalanced for the test condition to test the same number of mice from each condition on each day (for details, see Additional File 1: Data file). Immediately after the elevated plus maze test, blood was collected.

The fourth cohort of mice (n = 16) was used to measure basal levels of plasma corticosterone. The mice were either subjected to a behavioral test battery that did not include the elevated plus maze test in the same manner as in the first cohort of mice (n = 8) or were experimentally naïve (n = 8). The mice were 61–67 days old at the start of the test battery. Two days after the open field test, cages containing the mice with or without test battery experience (68–74 days old) were placed in the sound-attenuating room in which the elevated plus maze apparatus was set. Blood was collected from all the mice in a day in the same manner as the other cohorts of mice.

The naïve mice in all the cohorts had not been handled and were left undisturbed, except for routine cage maintenance conducted by the female laboratory technician. A male experimenter performed all the experiments, with the exception of the general health and neurological screen on test day 1 and the light/dark transition test on day 4 in the first cohort of mice that were carried out by the female technician. Consequently, the male experimenter conducted all procedures for the elevated plus maze test followed by blood sampling in all mice. In the elevated plus maze test, each mouse was picked up by the tail and removed from the home cage by the experimenter immediately before the test. The test-battery-experience group of the first cohort of mice received tail handling several times when subjected to a series of behavioral tests, whereas the other animals had not been habituated to tail handling before the elevated plus test.

Behavioral test

Cages containing the mice to be tested were moved from a housing room to the sound-attenuating room in the morning at least 30 min before starting the test. The cages were put into the top shelf of a rack (90 cm × 35 cm × 88 cm) on which a top plate made of matte black acrylic was placed to avoid direct lighting from fluorescent ceiling lights in the testing rooms (the illumination level in the cage was approximately 5 lx), with the exception of the third cohort of mice that was put in the rack outside the sound-attenuating room because their test was conducted at different illumination levels. In all the cohorts of mice, the elevated plus maze test was performed between 9:45 AM and 1:15 PM. In the first cohort, a series of behavioral tests that did not include the elevated plus maze test were performed between 9:00 AM and 2:30 PM. After each test, the floors and walls of the testing apparatuses were cleaned with hypochlorous acid water to eliminate any olfactory cues. The cages were returned to the housing room after each test.

General health and neurological screen

General health and neurological screen were conducted for 2 days. On day 1, physical characteristics and neurological reflexes (rectal temperature, body weight, righting reflex, whisker twitch reflex, ear twitch reflex, and visual placing reflex—a forepaw extension when lowered toward a visible surface) were assessed. To measure neuromuscular strength, mice were placed on a wire mesh that was then inverted, and the latency to fall from the mesh was recorded with a 60‐s cutoff time. On day 2, the forelimb grip strength was measured by holding the mice by their tails and lifting them so that their forepaws could grasp the wire grid of a grip strength meter (O'Hara & Co., Tokyo, Japan). The mice were gently pulled backward by the tail until they released the grid. The peak force applied by the forelimbs was recorded in newtons (N). Each mouse was tested three times. After the grip strength test, mice were placed in the box, and the reflex response to key jangling, the auditory stimulus, was recorded.

Light/dark transition test

The light/dark transition test was performed as previously described [48] on test day 4. Four apparatuses set up in one of the sound-attenuating rooms were used to test four mice in a cage simultaneously. The apparatus consisted of a cage (21 cm × 42 cm × 25 cm) divided into two sections of equal size by a partition with a door (O’Hara & Co.). One chamber consisted of white plastic walls and was brightly illuminated (390 lx) by LED lights attached above the ceiling of the chamber. The other chamber had black plastic walls and was dark (2 lx). Both chambers had a white plastic floor. Mice were placed into the dark chamber and were allowed to move freely between the two chambers for 10 min with the door open.

Open field test

The open field test was performed in an open field apparatus with the VersaMax Animal Activity Monitoring System (40 cm × 40 cm × 30 cm; Accuscan Instruments, Columbus, OH, USA). Eight apparatuses that were placed in a sound-attenuating room were used to test four to eight mice at the same time. The center area was illuminated to 100 lx by LED lights attached above the ceiling of each apparatus. The center area was defined as a 25 cm × 25 cm area. Each mouse was placed in one corner of the open field. Their behaviors, including the total distance traveled (cm), vertical activity (rearing, measured by counting the number of photobeam interruptions), time spent in the center area (s), and stereotypic counts (beam-break counts for stereotyped behaviors), were automatically recorded using an activity monitoring system for the entire 120-min period after the mice were placed in the apparatus.

Elevated plus maze test

The elevated plus maze test was performed as previously described [49], using one elevated plus maze apparatus that was placed in a sound-attenuating room. Eight to 12 mice per day were tested between 9:45 AM and 1:15 PM each day to avoid any possible influences of diurnal changes in plasma corticosterone levels. The apparatus consisted of two open arms (25 cm × 5 cm) and two closed arms of the same size with 15-cm-high walls and a central square (5 cm × 5 cm) connecting the arms (O’Hara & Co.). The floor of the arms and central square were made of white PVC plates and were elevated to a height of 55 cm above the floor. To prevent mice from falling off the open arms, the arms were surrounded by a raised ledge (3 mm thick and 3 mm high, composed of transparent blue PVC). Arms of the same type were located opposite one another. The walls of the closed arms were made from one of the following: clear transparent walls, bluish transparent walls, matte white opaque walls, and matte black opaque walls, which were made of PVC plates (O’Hara & Co.). In the experiments with the first and second cohorts, the illumination level was set to 100 lx at the center of the maze with the closed arms with transparent blue walls (for details, see Additional File 6: Table 1). For the experiment with the third cohort of mice, which was tested at different illumination levels (5, 100, or 800 lx; four infrared LED lights attached to the ceiling were used to allow a video image-based analysis under the dim light condition). In each test, the cage was placed on the top plate of the rack, and the cage lid was removed. Each mouse was gently picked up by the tail and placed in an empty plastic open cage (18.2 cm × 25.0 cm × 13.9 cm). The housing cage was put back into the shelf, and the open cage containing a mouse was close to the central platform of the maze. Each mouse was then picked up by the tail and placed in the central square of the maze facing one of the closed arms. Immediately after placement, the experimenter pushed a start button of a remote controller of a video image analysis system attached to the wall of the sound-attenuating room and left the testing room. A camera was mounted on the ceiling above the central platform of the maze. The distance traveled (cm), number of total arm entries, time spent in the closed arms (s), time spent in the center area (s), percentage of open arm entries, and percentage of time spent in the open arms were automatically measured during a 10-min test period using the ImageEP program (ImageJ-based program developed by Tsuyoshi Miyakawa and the source code are freely available from the Mouse Phenotype Database website, http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html, and from Git repository website, https://github.com/neuroinformatics [50]). Immediately following the end of the test, the experimenter returned to the room, put the mouse into the empty open cage, and then went to another sound-attenuating room adjacent to the testing room with the cage to take a blood sample. The floor and walls of the arms and central square were cleaned by hypochlorous acid water before each test to eliminate possible odors left by the different colored wall arms and the previous subject.

Plasma corticosterone measurement

In the first, second, and third cohorts of mice, immediately after the elevated plus maze test (within approximately 1 min), blood was collected from the facial vein or submandibular vein using a Goldenrod Animal Lancet (MEDIpoint, Inc., NY, USA) into tubes containing 1 unit of sodium heparin (Wako Pure Chemical Industries Ltd., Osaka, Japan) and kept on ice. In the fourth cohort of mice, at least one hour after acclimation to the testing room where an elevated plus maze apparatus was set, blood was collected to measure basal levels of plasma corticosterone. The blood samples were centrifuged at 3000×g for 10 min at 4 °C. Supernatants were collected and stored at − 80 °C until measurement. Plasma corticosterone (CORT) concentrations were determined using a correlate-enzyme immunoassay kit (Assay Designs Inc., MI, USA) according to the manufacturer's protocol.

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed with Student’s t-test in the first and fourth cohorts of mice and one-way ANOVAs in the second and third cohorts of mice. The post hoc comparisons were performed by Student’s t-test with Bonferroni correction. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients of the elevated plus maze behaviors with plasma corticosterone levels were calculated to examine relationships between the two measures. The significance level was set at 0.05. The statistical analysis was performed using SAS University Edition software (SAS Institute Inc., NC, USA).

Results

Mice with test battery experience showed slightly less distance traveled and fewer total arm entries (Fig. 1a: t30 = 1.78, p = 0.0849; Fig. 1b: t30 = 1.91, p = 0.0660, respectively), spent significantly more time in the closed arms (Fig. 1c: t30 = 5.06, p < 0.0001) but spent less time in the center area (Fig. 1d: t30 = 3.52, p = 0.0014), entered the open arms less frequently (Fig. 1e: t30 = 2.90, p = 0.0069), and spent less time in the open arms (Fig. 1f: t30 = 4.17, p = 0.0002) than mice with no prior experience. Plasma corticosterone concentrations after exposure to the elevated plus maze were lower in the test-battery-experience group of mice than in the group of mice with no prior experience (Fig. 1g: t30 = 2.64, p = 0.0129), while the basal plasma corticosterone levels in the mice with and without test battery experience did not differ from each other (Additional File 3: Fig. 2: t14 = 0.45, p = 0.6586). In this cohort of mice, the elevated plus maze test was conducted 2–4 days after the open field test, and therefore, whether the different intertest intervals contributed to behavioral differences between groups was examined. Two-way ANOVAs with group and test day as factors showed no significant group × test day interactions on any measures (Additional File 4: Fig. 3), which indicated that group differences in each behavior were not dependent on the test day.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Effects of test experience on behaviors and plasma corticosterone levels in the elevated plus maze in male C57BL/6J mice. Mice that were subjected to behavioral tests other than the elevated plus maze test (n = 16) and mice with no test experience (n = 16) were tested in an elevated plus maze. a Distance traveled (cm), b number of total arm entries, c time spent in closed arms (s), d time spent in center area (s), e percentage of open arm entries (%), and f percentage of time spent in open arms (%) were measured. g Plasma corticosterone levels (ng/mL) of the mice exposed to the elevated plus maze were determined. Values are the means ± SEM

For the second cohort of mice, there were significant effects of closed-arm wall color on several behavioral measures (Fig. 2a–f), including time spent in the closed arms (Fig. 2c: F3,44 = 5.92, p = 0.0017), time spent in the center area (Fig. 2d: F3,44 = 3.33, p = 0.0279), percentage of open arm entries (Fig. 2e: F3,44 = 5.37, p = 0.0031), and percentage of time spent in the open arms (Fig. 2f: F3,44 = 6.39, p = 0.0011). No significant effects of closed-arm wall color were found on distance traveled (Fig. 2a: F3,44 = 1.94, p = 0.1365), number of total arm entries (Fig. 2b: F3,44 = 0.22, p = 0.8852), or plasma corticosterone levels (Fig. 2g: F3,44 = 0.77, p = 0.5153). Post hoc comparisons showed that mice subjected to the black wall condition spent more time in the closed arms and exhibited lower percentages of open arm entries and open arm time than mice in the clear transparent condition (p = 0.0002, p = 0.0002, and p < 0.0001, respectively). Mice in the black wall condition also spent more time in the closed arms than mice in the white and bluish transparent wall conditions, although the difference between the black and bluish transparent wall conditions did not reach significance after Bonferroni correction (p = 0.0058 and p = 0.0383, respectively). Mice in the black wall condition spent less time in the center area than mice in the white wall condition (p = 0.0037). Mice in the clear transparent wall condition tended to enter the open arms more frequently and spend more time in the open arms than mice in the bluish transparent and white wall conditions (for percentage of open arm entries, clear vs. bluish transparent and white, p = 0.0323 and p = 0.0431; for percentage of open arm time, clear vs. bluish transparent and white, p = 0.0251 and p = 0.0052), although almost all the statistical results did not reach significance after Bonferroni correction.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Effects of closed arm wall color on behaviors and plasma corticosterone levels in the elevated plus maze in male C57BL/6J mice. Mice were tested in an elevated plus maze with closed arms of any one of four colors (clear, transparent blue, white, and black; n = 12 per test condition). a Distance traveled (cm), b number of total arm entries, c time spent in closed arms (s), d time spent in center area (s), e percentage of open arm entries (%), f percentage of time spent in open arms (%), and g plasma corticosterone concentration (ng/mL) after the test were measured. Values are the means ± SEM. When ANOVA indicated a significant effect (p < 0.05), group comparisons were made using Student's t-test: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 after Bonferroni correction

In the third cohort of mice, there were no significant effects of illumination level on any behavioral measures or plasma CORT level (Fig. 3a–g: distance traveled, F2,33 = 2.41, p = 0.1058; number of total arm entries, F2,33 = 3.09, p = 0.0588; time spent in closed arms, F2,33 = 1.34, p = 0.2750; time spent in center area, F2,33 = 0.95, p = 0.3968; percentage of open arm entries, F2,33 = 2.33, p = 0.1133; percentage of time spent in open arms, F2,33 = 0.84, p = 0.4415; plasma CORT level, F2,33 = 1.14, p = 0.3312).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Effects of illumination level on behaviors and plasma corticosterone levels in the elevated plus maze in male C57BL/6J mice. Mice were tested in the elevated plus maze under any one of three lighting conditions (illumination levels of 5, 100, and 800 lx, on the center area of the maze). a Distance traveled (cm), b number of total arm entries, c time spent in closed arms (s), d time spent in center area (s), e percentage of open arm entries (%), f percentage of time spent in open arms (%), and g plasma corticosterone concentration (ng/mL) after the test were measured. Values are the means ± SEM

To examine the relationships between the elevated plus maze behaviors and plasma corticosterone levels, Spearman’s rank correlation coefficients of each behavioral measure with plasma corticosterone levels were calculated using the data from the groups of mice tested under the different test conditions (for each group, n = 32–48; Additional File 5: Fig. 4a–r) and from all the mice (n = 116; Additional File 5: Fig. 4s–x). The correlation analyses showed that there was a significant negative correlation of plasma corticosterone level with time spent in the closed arms in the first cohort of mice with and without test experience (Additional File 5: Fig. 4c), while there were no any significant correlations between plasma corticosterone levels and behavioral measures in the group of all 116 mice (Additional File 5: Fig. 4s–x).

Discussion

The present study investigated the influences of experience with a battery of behavioral tests that did not include the elevated plus maze test, closed-arm wall color, and illumination level on elevated plus maze behavior during a 10-min period from the elevated plus maze test and plasma corticosterone levels immediately after exposure to the maze in male C57BL/6J mice. The results showed that experience with some types of behavioral tests induced decreases in both exploration of the open arms of the maze and plasma corticosterone response, while testing in closed arms of different wall colors affected behavior but not plasma corticosterone levels, and illumination level had no significant effects on any measures. Additionally, correlation analyses showed a negative correlation of close arm time with corticosterone levels in the cohort of mice with and without test experience, whereas in a group consisting of all mice used in this study, there were no significant correlations between them. These findings indicate that experience with other behavioral tests and test apparatus construction are significant factors affecting elevated plus maze behaviors, but the relationships between the behaviors and plasma corticosterone levels may depend on the test situation and thus remain unclear in C57BL/6J mice.

The present results suggest that reduction in open arm exploration can occur due to prior exposure to test apparatuses other than the elevated plus maze, which seems to be consistent with findings of previous studies [2, 24]. Our present study and other previous studies [2, 24] may provide an additional or alternative explanation for the reduction in open arm exploration in mice with prior experience in previous studies using the test/retest protocol, in which the reduction of open arm exploration in a retest was considered to be based on learning of open arm avoidance during the first elevated plus maze test [12, 13, 15, 19,20,21]. The reduced open arm exploration in the retest may be due to exposure to the elevated plus maze apparatus itself and/or due to previous experience(s), other than reexposure to the apparatus, such as being taken out of the cage, handling, etc. This conclusion may also be supported by a study reporting that behavior subsequent to prior maze experience was not affected by manipulations of extramaze cues (90° reorientation of the maze or use of a different laboratory) [12]. It would be unlikely that the reduction in open arm exploration was due to decreased general locomotor activity because mice with and without test experience did not significantly differ in the distance traveled and number of total arm entries. There was also little possibility that the reduced exploration could be produced by repeated handling by an experimenter during the test battery, as suggested by previous studies indicating that repeated handling increased open arm exploration [51, 52]. A simple explanation of the reduced open arm exploration and increased time spent in the closed arms is that test experience, irrespective of test type, induces an increased level of anxiety. However, given the potential anxiolytic effects of handling [51, 52] in addition to the decreased levels of plasma corticosterone stress response found in this study, it seems to be unlikely that the decreased exploratory behavior reflects increased level of anxiety. Mice with test battery experience showed decreased time spent in the center area, where novelty-seeking and risk-assessment behaviors could be observed [53,54,55]. These findings suggest another possibility: the decreased open arm exploration in the mice with test battery experience may be due to a reduction in novelty seeking. On the other hand, naïve mice with no prior test experience might exhibit intense anxiety-like behavior similar to panic-like reactions to escape from open arms [54] when confronted with a novel environment for the first time. In this study, the use of the method of picking mice by the tail before the test might also enhance stress and anxiety levels [56, 57], especially in naïve mice, resulting in higher levels of corticosterone stress responses. In our lab, we have analyzed behaviors of more than 200 mutant strains of mice using a battery of behavioral tests. Mice with mutations in genes, such as Calcineurin (CNB, also called protein phosphatase 2B, calcium and calmodulin dependent serine/threonine protein phosphatase), PACAP (pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide), PSD-95 (postsynaptic density protein 95), Hivep2 (human immunodeficiency virus type I enhancer binding protein 2, also known as Schnurri-2), Grin1 (glutamate ionotropic receptor NMDA type subunit 1), Reln (reelin), Dab1 (disabled 1), Ts1Cje (reciprocal translocation in chromosome 16), Caskin1 (CASK interacting protein 1), and Syngap1 (synaptic Ras GTPase activating protein 1), showed more open arm exploration behaviors than their wild-type control mice in the elevated plus maze test, while in the light/dark transition test the mutants exhibited decreases in the time spent in the light chamber and in the number of transitions between the light and dark chambers, which are in general considered to reflect increased anxiety, or no behavioral differences in the light/dark transition test compared to control mice [42, 58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66]. Such paradoxical results are also observed in middle-aged wild type mice compared to young mice [67]. These findings highlight the difficulty in interpreting the increased time and entries in open arms simply as decreased levels of anxiety in some cases and indicate the need for further study to explore what the behaviors mean.

Previous studies have indicated that exploration to the open arms was higher when tested in a maze with closed arms with acrylic transparent/translucent walls than with black acrylic/wooden opaque walls in rats [25,26,27] and mice [28]. The results of this study are consistent with the previous findings [25,26,27,28]. The present study further indicate the importance of the transparency and color of walls on elevated plus maze behavior: the most pronounced differences in the behaviors were found between the clear and black wall conditions, whereas such differences were not observed between the transparent blue wall and black wall conditions, and there were slight differences in open arm exploration between the two types of transparent wall arms and between the two types of opaque closed arms. These behavioral differences do not seem to be related to locomotor activity, since there were no significant wall color-induced changes in distance traveled and number of total arm entries. Rodents have a tendency to avoid open spaces by visual perception, allowing them to discriminate open and closed arms [7, 68]. Our results suggest that when less transparent and more dark-colored closed arms, especially black closed arms, are used, mice can more easily discriminate the open and closed arms and potentiate them to avoid the open arms.

It has been reported that animals tested under high illumination conditions show reduced exploratory behavior in open arms or increased anxiety-like behavior in several previous studies comparing different illumination conditions on the maze floor (20 lx vs. 1200 lx [29]) and on the central platform (44 lx vs. 600 lx [23]; 0, 1, 3, 10, 30, 100, and 300 lx [31]), on the arms (80 and 90 lx on open and closed arms vs. 220 and 110 lx on open and closed arms [30]; 4 lx on all arms vs. 210 and 60 lx on open and closed arms [69]), and in the testing room (0.1 footcandle light condition vs. 20.0 footcandle light condition [32]), while some reports have shown no behavioral differences between animals tested under 30, 300, and 900 lx on arms [33] and 9 and 297 lx in the testing room [34]. Such inconsistent findings may be partially explained by differences in illumination levels between the open and closed arms; a study showed that the difference in illumination levels between the open and closed arms is a determinant of open arm exploration [70], and therefore, lack of behavioral differences between the different lighting conditions [33] might be explained as a consequence of the same illumination levels in all the arms in each lighting condition for rats [70]. The present study showed no significant behavioral differences between the groups of mice tested under the three lighting conditions, although the differences in illumination levels between the open and closed arms differed for the 5-, 100-, and 800-lx illumination conditions (see Additional File 6: Table 1) in C57BL/6J mice. These findings suggest the need to further investigate the effects of illumination levels on elevated plus maze behaviors in mice while considering possible methodological differences between studies, such as species, strain, prior experience, and apparatus construction.

The acute effects of corticosterone on anxiety-related behavior remain controversial, since a single corticosterone administration has shown to have an anxiogenic- and anxiolytic-like effects in the elevated plus maze test and the light/dark transition test [71,72,73]. A correlation study reported that plasma corticosterone levels in response to exposure to the elevated plus maze were correlated with risk-assessment behaviors and time spent in closed arms in Swiss-Webster mice [39]. However, the present study showed that, although a significant correlation between plasma corticosterone levels and time spent in closed arms was found in the cohort of mice with and without prior test experience, our data from the total group of 116 mice revealed no correlations of plasma corticosterone level with behavior. These observations suggest that there are no clear relationships between the two measures in C57BL/6J mice. One possible limitation of this study is the use of the 10-min test protocol. Considering the higher corticosterone levels in rats exposed to the maze for the 10-min period compared with the 5-min period [37], the lack of correlations in this study might be due to a possible ceiling effect by the 10-min exposure to the maze on corticosterone levels.

The present study reveals that previous experience with certain behavioral tests result in alterations in both elevated plus maze behaviors and plasma corticosterone response, and the closed arm type but not illumination level also had significant effects on behaviors in the elevated plus maze, in C57BL/6J mice. These results emphasize the need for careful consideration of test conditions in interpreting the behavioral outcomes under different test conditions of the elevated plus maze. One limitation of this study was that only male mice were used. It will be necessary to consider the use of females in designing experiments to further understand the influences of various variables, including sex and their interactions, on elevated plus maze behavior. Importantly, higher percentages of open arm time and entries are conventionally interpreted as decreased levels of anxiety. As discussed above, there is the possibility that the increased time and entries in open arms may rather reflect a heightened state of anxiety, escape behavior, and panic-like response to a novel environment in some specific cases. These findings suggest the need to measure anxiety-like behaviors under various situations with different types of tests to avoid misinterpretation and generalization from a single test.

Availability of data and materials

All the data directly associated with the results of this study are included in the Data file. The data from the behavioral tests are also accessible through the online database “Mouse Phenotype Database” (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/).

References

  1. Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open: closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods. 1985;14:149–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology. 1987;92:180–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Dawson GR, Tricklebank MD. Use of the elevated plus maze in the search for novel anxiolytic agents. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1995;16:33–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Hogg S. A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1996;54:21–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Rodgers RJ, Dalvi A. Anxiety, defence and the elevated plus-maze. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1997;21:801–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Walf AA, Frye CA. The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat Protoc. 2007;2:322–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Treit D, Menard J, Royan C. Anxiogenic stimuli in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1993;44:463–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Carobrez AP, Bertoglio LJ. Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:1193–205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Rodgers RJ, Shepherd JK. Influence of prior maze experience on behaviour and response to diazepam in the elevated plus-maze and light/dark tests of anxiety in mice. Psychopharmacology. 1993;113:237–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Dawson GR, Crawford SP, Stanhope KJ, Iversen SD, Tricklebank MD. One-trial tolerance to the effects of chlordiazepoxide on the elevated plus maze may be due to locomotor habituation, not repeated drug exposure. Psychopharmacology. 1994;113:570–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Espejo EF. Effects of weekly or daily exposure to the elevated plus-maze in male mice. Behav Brain Res. 1997;87:233–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Rodgers RJ, Johnson NJT, Carr J, Hodgson TP. Resistance of experientially-induced changes in murine plus-maze behaviour to altered retest conditions. Behav Brain Res. 1997;86:71–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Holmes A, Rodgers RJ. Responses of Swiss-Webster mice to repeated plus-maze experience: further evidence for a qualitative shift in emotional state? Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;60:473–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Pereira JKD, Vieira RJ, Konishi CT, Ribeiro RDA, Frussa-Filho R. The phenomenon of “one-trial tolerance” to the anxiolytic effect of chlordiazepoxide in the elevated plus-maze is abolished by the introduction of a motivational conflict situation. Life Sci. 1999;65:PL101–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP. Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety. Behav Brain Res. 2000;108:197–203.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. File SE. One-trial tolerance to the anxiolytic effects of chlordiazepoxide in the plus-maze. Psychopharmacology. 1990;100:281–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. File SE. The interplay of learning and anxiety in the elevated plus-maze. Behav Brain Res. 1993;58:199–202.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. File SE, Mabbutt PS, Hitchcott PK. Characterisation of the phenomenon of “one-trial tolerance” to the anxiolytic effect of chlordiazepoxide in the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacology. 1990;102:98–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. File SE, Zangrossi H, Viana M, Graeff FG. Trial 2 in the elevated plus-maze: a different form of fear? Psychopharmacology. 1993;111:491–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Dal-Col MLC, Orlandi-Pereira L, Rosa VP, Calixto AV, Carobrez AP, Faria MS. Lack of midazolam-induced anxiolysis in the plus-maze Trial 2 is dependent on the length of Trial 1. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;74:395–400.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP. Scopolamine given pre-Trial 1 prevents the one-trial tolerance phenomenon in the elevated plus-maze Trial 2. Behav Pharmacol. 2004;15:45–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gonzalez LE, File SE. A five-minute experience in the elevated plus-maze alters the state of the benzodiazepine receptor in the dorsal raphe nucleus. J Neurosci. 1997;17:1505–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Bertoglio LJ, Carobrez AP. Behavioral profile of rats submitted to session 1-session 2 in the elevated plus-maze during diurnal/nocturnal phases and under different illumination conditions. Behav Brain Res. 2002;132:135–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Voikar V, Vasar E, Rauvala H. Behavioral alterations induced by repeated testing in C57BL/6J and 129S2/Sv mice: implications for phenotyping screens. Genes Brain Behav. 2004;3:27–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Violle N, Balandras F, Le Roux Y, Desor D, Schroeder H. Variations in illumination, closed wall transparency and/or extramaze space influence both baseline anxiety and response to diazepam in the rat elevated plus-maze. Behav Brain Res. 2009;203:35–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Anseloni VZ, Motta V, Lima G, Brandao ML. Behavioral and pharmacological validation of the elevated plus maze constructed with transparent walls. Braz J Med Biol Res. 1995;28:597–601.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Horii Y, Kawaguchi M. Higher detection sensitivity of anxiolytic effects of diazepam by ledge-free open arm with opaque walled closed arm elevated plus maze in male rats. Behav Brain Res. 2015;294:131–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hagenbuch N, Feldon J, Yee BK. Use of the elevated plus-maze test with opaque or transparent walls in the detection of mouse strain differences and the anxiolytic effects of diazepam. Behav Pharmacol. 2006;17:31–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Morato S, Castrechini P. Effects of floor surface and environmental illumination on exploratory activity in the elevated plus-maze. Braz J Med Biol Res. 1989;22:707–10.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Griebel G, Moreau JL, Jenck F, Martin JR, Misslin R. Some critical determinants of the behaviour of rats in the elevated plus-maze. Behav Proc. 1993;29:37–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Garcia AMB, Cardenas FP, Morato S. Effect of different illumination levels on rat behavior in the elevated plus-maze. Physiol Behav. 2005;85:265–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Benjamin D, Lal H, Meyerson LR. The effects of 5-HT1B characterizing agents in the mouse elevated plus-maze. Life Sci. 1990;47:195–203.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Becker A, Grecksch G. Illumination has no effect on rats’ behavior in the elevated plus-maze. Physiol Behav. 1996;59:1175–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Jones N, King SM. Influence of circadian phase and test illumination on pre-clinical models of anxiety. Physiol Behav. 2001;72:99–106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. McEwen BS, Bowles NP, Gray JD, Hill MN, Hunter RG, Karatsoreos IN, Nasca C. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1353.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. File SE, Johnston AL, Baldwin HA. Anxiolytic and anxiogenic drugs: changes in behaviour and endocrine responses. Stress Med. 1988;4:221–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. File SE, Zangrossi H, Sanders FL, Mabbutt PS. Raised corticosterone in the rat after exposure to the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacology. 1994;113:543–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Calvo N, Martijena ID, Molina VA, Volosin M. Metyrapone pretreatment prevents the behavioral and neurochemical sequelae induced by stress. Brain Res. 1998;800:227–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Rodgers RJ, Haller J, Holmes A, Halasz J, Walton TJ, Brain PF. Corticosterone response to the plus-maze: high correlation with risk assessment in rats and mice. Physiol Behav. 1999;68:47–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Nakao A, Miki T, Shoji H, Nishi M, Takeshima H, Miyakawa T, Mori Y. Comprehensive behavioral analysis of voltage-gated calcium channel beta-anchoring and-regulatory protein knockout mice. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:141.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Katayama Y, Nishiyama M, Shoji H, Ohkawa Y, Kawamura A, Sato T, Suyama M, Takumi T, Miyakawa T, Nakayama KI. CHD8 haploinsufficiency results in autistic-like phenotypes in mice. Nature. 2016;537:675–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Imai H, Shoji H, Ogata M, Kagawa Y, Owada Y, Miyakawa T, Sakimura K, Terashima T, Katsuyama Y. Dorsal forebrain-specific deficiency of reelin-dab1 signal causes behavioral abnormalities related to psychiatric disorders. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27:3485–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Shoji H, Miyakawa T. Age-related behavioral changes from young to old age in male mice of a C57BL/6J strain maintained under a genetic stability program. Neuropsychopharmacol Rep. 2019;39:100–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Crawley JN, Paylor R. A proposed test battery and constellations of specific behavioral paradigms to investigate the behavioral phenotypes of transgenic and knockout mice. Horm Behav. 1997;31:197–211.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Metz GA, Schwab ME. Behavioral characterization in a comprehensive mouse test battery reveals motor and sensory impairments in growth-associated protein-43 null mutant mice. Neuroscience. 2004;129:563–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Duffy L, Cappas E, Scimone A, Schofield PR, Karl T. Behavioral profile of a heterozygous mutant mouse model for EGF-like domain neuregulin 1. Behav Neurosci. 2008;122:748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Nishino T, Tamada K, Maeda A, Abe T, Kiyonari H, Funahashi Y, Kaibuchi K, Takumi T, Konishi H. Behavioral analysis in mice deficient for GAREM2 (Grb2-associated regulator of Erk/MAPK subtype2) that is a subtype of highly expressing in the brain. Mol Brain. 2019;12:94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Takao K, Miyakawa T. Light/dark transition test for mice. J Vis Exp. 2006;1:e104.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Komada M, Takao K, Miyakawa T. Elevated plus maze for mice. J Vis Exp. 2008;22:e1088.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Hattori S, Okumura Y, Takao K, Yamaguchi Y, Miyakawa T. Open source code for behavior analysis in rodents. Neuropsychopharmacol Rep. 2019;39:67–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Schmitt U, Hiemke C. Strain differences in open-field and elevated plus-maze behavior of rats without and with pretest handling. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;59:807–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Ueno H, Takahashi Y, Suemitsu S, Murakami S, Kitamura N, Wani K, Matsumoto Y, Okamoto M, Ishihara T. Effects of repetitive gentle handling of male C57BL/6NCrl mice on comparative behavioural test results. Sci Rep. 2020;10:1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Cruz ADM, Frei F, Graeff FG. Ethopharmacological analysis of rat behavior on the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994;49:171–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Holmes A, Parmigiani S, Ferrari PF, Palanza P, Rodgers RJ. Behavioral profile of wild mice in the elevated plus-maze test for anxiety. Physiol Behav. 2000;71:509–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Wall PM, Messier C. Methodological and conceptual issues in the use of the elevated plus-maze as a psychological measurement instrument of animal anxiety-like behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:275–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Hurst JL, West RS. Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat Methods. 2010;7:825–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Gouveia K, Hurst JL. Reducing mouse anxiety during handling: effect of experience with handling tunnels. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e66401.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Miyakawa T, Leiter LM, Gerber DJ, Gainetdinov RR, Sotnikova TD, Zeng H, Caron MG, Tonegawa S. Conditional calcineurin knockout mice exhibit multiple abnormal behaviors related to schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:8987–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Hattori S, Takao K, Tanda K, Toyama K, Shintani N, Baba A, Hashimoto H, Miyakawa T. Comprehensive behavioral analysis of pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide (PACAP) knockout mice. Front Behav Neurosci. 2012;6:58.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Nagura H, Ishikawa Y, Kobayashi K, Takao K, Tanaka T, Nishikawa K, Tamura H, Shiosaka S, Suzuki H, Miyakawa T, Fujiyoshi Y, Doi T. Impaired synaptic clustering of postsynaptic density proteins and altered signal transmission in hippocampal neurons, and disrupted learning behavior in PDZ1 and PDZ2 ligand binding-deficient PSD-95 knockin mice. Mol Brain. 2012;5:43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Takao K, Kobayashi K, Hagihara H, Ohira K, Shoji H, Hattori S, Koshimizu H, Umemori J, Toyama K, Nakamura HK, Kuroiwa M, Maeda J, Atsuzawa K, Esaki K, Yamaguchi S, Furuya S, Takagi T, Walton NM, Hayashi N, Suzuki H, Higuchi M, Usuda N, Suhara T, Nishi A, Matsumoto M, Ishii S, Miyakawa M. Deficiency of schnurri-2, an MHC enhancer binding protein, induces mild chronic inflammation in the brain and confers molecular, neuronal, and behavioral phenotypes related to schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:1409–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Umemori J, Takao K, Koshimizu H, Hattori S, Furuse T, Wakana S, Miyakawa T. ENU-mutagenesis mice with a non-synonymous mutation in Grin1 exhibit abnormal anxiety-like behaviors, impaired fear memory, and decreased acoustic startle response. BMC Res Notes. 2013;6:203.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Sakai K, Shoji H, Kohno T, Miyakawa T, Hattori M. Mice that lack the C-terminal region of Reelin exhibit behavioral abnormalities related to neuropsychiatric disorders. Sci Rep. 2016;6:28636.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Shimohata A, Ishihara K, Hattori S, Miyamoto H, Morishita H, Ornthanalai G, Raveau M, Ebrahim AS, Amano K, Yamada K, Sago H, Akiba S, Mataga N, Murphy NP, Miyakawa T, Yamakawa K. Ts1Cje Down syndrome model mice exhibit environmental stimuli-triggered locomotor hyperactivity and sociability concurrent with increased flux through central dopamine and serotonin metabolism. Exp Neurol. 2017;293:1–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Katano T, Takao K, Abe M, Yamazaki M, Watanabe M, Miyakawa T, Sakimura K, Ito S. Distribution of Caskin1 protein and phenotypic characterization of its knockout mice using a comprehensive behavioral test battery. Mol Brain. 2018;11:1–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Nakajima R, Takao K, Hattori S, Shoji H, Komiyama NH, Grant SG, Miyakawa T. Comprehensive behavioral analysis of heterozygous Syngap1 knockout mice. Neuropsychopharmacol Rep. 2019;39:223–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Shoji H, Takao K, Hattori S, Miyakawa T. Age-related changes in behavior in C57BL/6J mice from young adulthood to middle age. Mol Brain. 2016;9:1–18.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Martinez JC, Cardenas F, Lamprea M, Morato S. The role of vision and proprioception in the aversion of rats to the open arms of an elevated plus-maze. Behav Proc. 2002;60:15–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Bert B, Fink H, Huston JP, Voits M. Fischer 344 and Wistar rats differ in anxiety and habituation but not in water maze performance. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2002;78(1):11–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Pereira LO, da Cunha IC, Neto JM, Paschoalini MA, Faria MS. The gradient of luminosity between open/enclosed arms, and not the absolute level of Lux, predicts the behaviour of rats in the plus maze. Behav Brain Res. 2005;159:55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Andreatini R, Leite J. The effect of corticosterone in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze and to to pentylenetetrazol-induced convulsions. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1994;18:1333–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  72. Ardayfio P, Kim KS. Anxiogenic-like effect of chronic corticosterone in the light-dark emergence task in mice. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:249–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Mitra R, Sapolsky RM. Acute corticosterone treatment is sufficient to induce anxiety and amygdaloid dendritic hypertrophy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:5573–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Tamaki Murakami for the assistance in animal husbandry and behavioral experiments.

Funding

This study was supported by JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Number JP 16H06276) and AMED (Grant Number JP20dm0107101). Behavioral analysis was carried out at the Institute for Comprehensive Medical Science, Fujita Health University, by the Joint Usage/Research Center for Genes, Brain and Behavior, which is accredited by the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports and Culture.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HS designed the study. HS conducted the experiments and analyzed the data. HS and TM wrote the manuscript. Both authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tsuyoshi Miyakawa.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All of the experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Fujita Health University.

Consent for publication

All authors consent to publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional File 1:

Data file

Additional File 2: Fig. 1.

Apparatus for the elevated plus maze test. (a) Elevated plus maze apparatus and (b) closed arms with different wall colors; clear, transparent blue, white, and black.

Additional File 3: Fig. 2.

Basal plasma corticosterone levels in male C57BL/6J mice with or without test experience. Male C57BL/6J mice were subjected to a series of behavioral tests, including an assessment of general health and neurological function, a light/dark transition test, and an open field test. Two days after the open field test, blood was collected from mice with test battery experience (n =8) and from naïve mice with no test experience (n = 8). All of the mice were left undisturbed in their home cages before blood collection. Plasma corticosterone concentrations (ng/mL) were measured. Values are the means ± SEM.

Additional File 4: Fig. 3.

The effects of intertest interval on behaviors in the elevated plus maze in male C57BL/6J mice. In the first cohort of mice, the test-battery-experience group (n = 16) was subjected to the elevated plus maze test 2, 3, or 4 days after the open field test (the test battery was started on test day 1; open field test was on test day 6; elevated plus maze test was on test day 8, n = 4; test day 9, n = 8; test day 10, n = 4). The naïve mice in the no-test-experience group (n = 16) were tested in the same manner as the test-battery-experience group on the test days (test day 8, n = 8; test day 9, n = 4; test day 10, n = 4). Values are the means ± SEM.

Additional File 5: Fig. 4.

Relationships between elevated plus maze behaviors and plasma corticosterone levels in male C57BL/6J mice. Scatterplots of elevated plus maze behaviors and plasma corticosterone levels in mice with or without test experience (a–f; n = 32 in total), mice tested in the maze with closed arms with different colored walls (g–l; n = 48 in total), mice tested under different illumination levels (m–r; n = 36 in total), and all mice (s–x; n = 116 in total). Correlations (Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient or rs, and p value) of plasma corticosterone levels with (a, g, m, s) distance traveled (cm), (b, h, n, t) number of total arm entries, (c, i, o, u) time spent in closed arms (s), (d, j, p, v) time spent in center (s), (e, k, q, w) percentage of open arm entries (%), and (f, l, r, x) percentage of open arm time (%) were calculated.

Additional File 6: Table 1.

Illumination levels in the elevated plus maze of closed arms with different color walls and under different light conditions. For the second group of mice, the illumination level was set to 100 lx at the center of the maze with closed arms with transparent blue walls. Measurements of illumination levels at each location of the maze (central platform, end of the open arms, and end of the closed arms) were repeated three times, and the average was calculated. For the third group of mice, the illumination level was set to 5, 100, or 800 lx at the center of the maze with closed arms with transparent blue walls. *The measurement sensor of the illuminometer was placed on the floor of the central platform and the end of the floor of the open arm horizontally. To measure the illumination level in the closed arm, the sensor was tilted due to the size of the illuminometer.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shoji, H., Miyakawa, T. Effects of test experience, closed-arm wall color, and illumination level on behavior and plasma corticosterone response in an elevated plus maze in male C57BL/6J mice: a challenge against conventional interpretation of the test. Mol Brain 14, 34 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13041-020-00721-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13041-020-00721-2

Keywords